Bridging languages for the constructed realities of different scholarly domains
Abstract
The search for a single interdisciplinary language for science, philosophy and religious studies is doomed to failure. Rather than the coarse granularity of these three fields, we focus on scholarly domains and demonstrate that even translation between a pair of domain languages may be impossible. Each domain language must support description of observations and theory, and provide satisfaction criteria for results asserted within the domain. Crucially, interaction between domains is based on human interaction, whether directly or through documents or artefacts. Thus we explore the relation between the mind of the individual scholar and the emerging consensuses that define a domain. The framework is based on Hesse and Arbib’s The Construction of Reality (CoR), extending a theory of “schemas in the head” to address the “social schemas” of a community. Members of a specific community -- such as a group of scholars within the same domain – may in some cases reach near agreement on the usage of terminology, but much scholarship is centered on disagreements, and these are magnified across different domains. Conversations between scholars in two domains requires a bridging language in which scholars may reach shared understandings of the terms each uses and thus reach shared conclusions, or agree to disagree. Developing bridging languages across domains of scholarly study may make new research questions arise and hence new domains of scholarly study may emerge – as exemplified by a case study bringing together linguistics, psychology, and neuroscience in the cognitive neuroscience of linguistics.
References
Arbib, M. A. (1972), The Metaphorical Brain: An Introduction to Cybernetics as Artificial Intelligence and Brain Theory, Wiley-Interscience, New York.
Arbib, M. A. (1975), «Artificial intelligence and brain theory: unities and diversities», in Ann Biomed Eng, 3(3), pp. 238-274, http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?cmd=Retrieve&db=PubMed&dopt=Citation&list_uids=1220582
Arbib, M. A. (1984) From Synergies and Embryos to Motor Schemas, (Chapter VIa), in H. T. A. Whiting, eds., Human Motor Actions: Bernstein Reassessed (pp. 545-562), Elsevier Science Publishers B.V., North-Holland.
Arbib, M. A. (1989), The Metaphorical Brain 2: Neural Networks and Beyond, Wiley-Interscience, New York.
Arbib, M. A. (1991), Neurons, Schemas, Persons, and Society, in J. E. Earley, eds., Individuality and Cooperative Action, pp. 63-86 (see also General Discussion, pp. 179-180), Georgetown University Press, Washington.
Arbib, M. A. (1999a), Crusoe's Brain: Of Solitude and Society, in R. J. Russell, N. Murphy, T.C. Meyering, & M. A. Arbib, eds., Neuroscience and the Person: Scientific Perspectives on Divine Action (pp. 419-448), Vatican Observatory Publications/Center for Theology and the Natural Sciences, Berkeley.
Arbib, M. A. (1999b), Towards a Neuroscience of the Person, in R. J. Russell, N. Murphy, T. C. Meyering, & M. A. Arbib, eds., Neuroscience and the Person: Scientific Perspectives on Divine Action (pp. 77-100), Vatican Observatory Publications/Center for Theology and the Natural Sciences, Berkeley.
Arbib, M. A. (2012), How the Brain Got Language: The Mirror System Hypothesis, Oxford University Press, Oxford.
Arbib, M. A. (2017), «Dorsal and ventral streams in the evolution of the language-ready brain: Linking language to the world», Journal of Neurolinguistics, 43, Part B, pp. 228-253, http://dx.doi.org/210.1016/j.jneuroling.2016.1012.1003.
Arbib, M. A. (2021), When Brains Meet Buildings: A Conversation between Neuroscience and Architecture, Oxford University Press, Oxford.
Arbib, M. A. (2022), Mary Hesse and “The Construction of Reality”. A celebration of the life of Professor Mary Hesse, www.researchgate.net/publication/368242592_Mary_Hesse_and_The_Construction_of_Reality.
Arbib, M. A. (2023), Atmosphere, Symbolism and Narrative: Towards a Neuroscience of Sacred Architecture (Unpublished), a talk presented at the conference on Neurophenomenology & Sacred Architecture: Toward an Experimental Theological Aesthetics, School of Architecture and Planning, Catholic University of America, Washington, D.C.
Arbib, M. A., & Hesse, M. B. (1986), The Construction of Reality, Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.
Arbib, M. A., & Lee, J. Y. (2008), «Describing visual scenes: Towards a neurolinguistics based on construction grammar», Brain Research, 1225, pp. 146-162, (last accessed 1st June 2023), http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?cmd=Retrieve&db=PubMed&dopt=Citation&list_uids=18561901
Barrès, V. (2017), Schema Architecture for Language-Vision InterActions: A Computational Cognitive Neuroscience Model of Language Use, Ph.D. Thesis, Computer Science, University of Southern California, Los Angeles.
Barrès, V., & Lee, J. Y. (2014), «Template Construction Grammar: From Visual Scene Description to Language Comprehension and Agrammatism», in Neuroinformatics, 12(1), pp. 181-208, (last accessed 1st June 2023), https://doi.org/10.1007/s12021-013-9197-y
Bartlett, F. C. (1932), Remembering. A Study in Experimental and Social Psychology, Cambridge University Press, Cambride.
Bernstein, N. A. (1967), The Coordination and Regulation of Movement, (trans. from the Russian), Pergamon Press, Oxford
Beth, E. W., & Piaget, J. (1966), Mathematical Epistemology and Psychology (Translated from the French by W. Mays), Springer Netherlands, Heidelberg.
Bornkessel-Schlesewsky, I., & Schlesewsky, M. (2013), «Reconciling time, space and function: A new dorsal–ventral stream model of sentence comprehension», Brain Lang, 125(1), pp. 60-76, https://doi.org/http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2013.01.010
Broca, P. (1861), «Remarques sur le siège de la faculté du langage articulé, suivies d'une observation d'aphémie (perte de la parole)», Bulletins de la Societé Anthropologique de Paris, 2, pp. 235-238.
Brothers, L. (1997), Friday’s footprint: how society shapes the human mind, Oxford University Press, Oxford.
Craik, K. J. W. (1943), The Nature of Explanation, Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.
Croft, W. (2001), Radical construction grammar: syntactic theory in typological perspective, Oxford University Press, Oxford.
Duffin, J. (2007), «The Doctor Was Surprised; or, How to Diagnose a Miracle», Bulletin of the History of Medicine, 81(4), pp. 699-729, https://doi.org/Project MUSE, doi:10.1353/bhm.2007.0124.
Durkheim, E. (1915), Elementary Forms of the Religious Life: A Study in Religious Sociology (Translated from the French original of 1912 by Joseph Ward Swain), Pallgrave Macmillan, London.
Ellis, G. F. R. (1999), Intimations of Transcendence: Relations of the mind to God, in R. J. Russell, N. Murphy, T. C. Meyering, & M. A. Arbib, eds., Neuroscience and the Person: Scientific Perspectives on Divine Action, Vatican Observatory Publications/Center for Theology and the Natural Sciences, Berkeley, pp. 449-474,
Freud, S. (1895), Project for a Scientific Psychology, in M. Bonaparte, A. Freud, & E. Kris, eds., The Origins of Psychoanalysis. Letters to Wilhelm Fleiss, Drafts and Notes: 1887-1902, Basic Books, New York.
Gadamer, H.-G. (1975), Truth and Method (English Translation), Sheed and Ward, New York.
Goldberg, A. E. (1995), Constructions: a Construction Grammar approach to argument structure, The University of Chicago Press, Chicago.
Goodfield, J. (1984), «Dr. Coley's toxins», Science '84, 5, 68+, (last accessed 1st June 2023), https://link.gale.com/apps/doc/A3205773/AONE?u=anon~49483959&sid=googleScholar&xid=f54f8652
Haruno, M., Wolpert, D. M., & Kawato, M. (2003), «Hierarchical MOSAIC for movement generation», International Congress Series, 1250, pp. 575-590, (last accessed 1st June 2023), https://doi.org/10.1016/s0531-5131(03)00190-0
Head, H., & Holmes, G. (1911), «Sensory Disturbances from Cerebral Lesions», Brain, 34, pp: 102-254.
Hesse, M. B. (1962), Forces and fields: the concept of action at a distance in the history of physics, SCM Press, London.
Hesse, M. B. (1963), Models and analogies in science, Sheed and Ward, New York.
Hesse, M. B. (1969), «Duhem, Quine and a new empiricism», Royal Institute of Philosophy Supplements, 3, pp. 191-209.
Hesse, M. B. (1974). The structure of scientific inference. Univ of California Press.
Hesse, M. B. (1980), Revolutions and Reconstructions in the Philosophy of Science., Indiana University Press, Bloomington.
Knowlson, J. (1975), Universal language schemes in England and France 1600-1800, University of Toronto Press, Toronto.
Kuhn, T. S. (1962), The Structure of Scientific Revolutions, University of Chicago Press, Chicago.
Lahiri, J. (2016), In Other Words, Bloomsbury, London.
Lahiri, J. (2022), Translating Myself and Others, Princeton University Press, Princeton.
Laland, K. N., Odling-Smee, F. J., & Feldman, M. W. (1996), «The evolutionary consequences of niche construction: A theoretical investigation using two-locus theory», J. Evol. Biol, 9(3), pp. 293–316.
Lee, J. Y. (2012), Linking Eyes to Mouth: A Schema-based Computational Model for Describing Visual Scenes, Ph.D. Thesis, Computer Science, University of Southern California, Los Angeles, CA.
Leslie, A. M. (1987), «Pretense and representation in infancy: the origins of "theory of mind"». Psychol Rev, 94, pp. 84-106.
Lichtheim, L. (1885), «On aphasia». Brain, 7, pp. 433-484.
Meltzoff, A. N. (2005), Imitation and other minds: the ‘‘like me’’ hypothesis, in S. Hurley & N. Chater, eds., Perspectives on Imitation, The MIT Press.
Odling-Smee, F. J., Laland, K. N., & Feldman, M. W. (2003). Niche construction: The neglected process in evolution. Princeton University Press, Cambridge, Massachussets, pp. 55-78.
Piaget, J. (1954), The construction of reality in the child, Norton, New York.
Piaget, J. (1971), Biology and knowledge: An essay on the relations between organic regulations and cognitive processes, Edinburgh University Press, Edinburgh (Biologie et connaissance: Essai sur les relations entre les régulations organiques et les processus cognitifs, Gallimard, Paris, 1967).
Reiss, R. (Ed.). (1964). Neural Theory and Modelling, Stanford University Press, Redwood City.
Schmidt, R. A. (1975), «A Schema Theory of Discrete Motor Skill Learning», Psychological Review, 82, pp. 225-260.
Severance, A. D. (1912), «Beatification and Canonization with Special Reference to Historic Proof and the Proof of Miracles», Papers of the American Society of Church History, 3, pp. 41-62, https://doi.org/10.1017/S1079902800114160
Sluga, H. (2011), Wittgenstein, Wiley, New York.
Tsung, K., & Norton, J. A. (2006), «Lessons from Coley's Toxin», Surgical Oncology, 15(1), pp. 25-28, https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1016/j.suronc.2006.05.002
Van der Burght, C. L., Friederici, A. D., Maran, M., Papitto, G., Pyatigorskaya, E., Schroën, J. A. M., Trettenbrein, P. C., & Zaccarella, E. (2022), Cleaning up the Brickyard: How Theory and Methodology Affect Experimental Outcome in Cognitive Neuroscience of Language. Preprint, DOI: 10.31234/osf.io/6zpjq.
Vidal, F. (2007), «Miracles, Science, and Testimony in Post-Tridentine Saint-Making», Science in Context, 20(3), pp. 481-508, https://doi.org/10.1017/S0269889707001391
Volterra, V., Capirci, O., Rinaldi, P., & Sparaci, L. (2018), «From action to spoken and signed language through gesture: some basic issues for a discussion on the evolution of the human language-ready brain», Interaction Studies, 19(1-2).
Wernicke, C. (1874), Der aphasische symptomencomplex, Cohn and Weigert, Breslau.
Wiener, N. (1948), Cybernetics: or Control and Communication in the Animal and the Machine, The Technology Press and John Wiley & Sons, New York.
Zukow-Goldring, P., & Arbib, M. A. (2007), «Affordances, effectivities, and assisted imitation: Caregivers and the directing of attention», Neurocomputing, 70, pp. 2181–2193.
Works published in RIFL are released under Creative Commons Licence:Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives 4.0 International License.