Il gesto come risorsa bio-linguistica

  • Sabina Fontana
  • Elena Mignosi

Abstract

Research on the role played by gesture in language development have supported the hypothesis of a strong interconnection between language and movement (KIMURA 1981, MCNEILL 1992, PÜLVERMUELLER et al. 1996, IVERSON & THELEN 1999, BATES & FREDERIC 2002). Motor actions of hand and mouth are present from birth but this link becomes significant as a result of shared social meaning. In early infancy, hand-to-mouth behavior are extremely common in infants’ spontaneous movements and occur similarly both in hearing and deaf children (see FONTANA & VOLTERRA 2012 for a review). Furthermore, similar motor constraints have been found in the production of gestures and a correlation between first actions and gestures in hearing and deaf toddlers has been pointed out in early language development (GALLESE & LAKOFF 2005, CAPIRCI et al. 2005, PETTENATI et al. 2010). Action and gestures emerge as a biological link but soon acquire social meaning through action routines which establish and fix a convention (BATES 1979). Transition from gesture to language occurs through the semiotic relationship between the caregiver and the child. In other words, what activates language is the relationship between child and adult. It has its origins in the biological sociality of human nature (STERN 1985, 2010). As result of the child’s need of communication, gestures enable communication through relation and participation to social interactions and routines. The role of social interaction and relation with caregivers will be analyzed by referring also to the Theory of Embodied Simulation (GALLESE & SINIGAGLIA 2011) in order to show its importance in the transition from spontaneous movement to social and shared action and finally to the onset of communication made of gestures and vocalizations.

References

AJURIAGUERRA J. de (1971), Manuel de Psychiatrie de l’enfant 2a ed., Paris, Masson & C., 1971 (Manuale di psichiatria del bambino, trad. it., Milano, Masson, 1979).

ANZIEU D. et. al. (1977), Psychanalyse et langage: du corps à la parole, Paris, Dunod (Psicoanalisi e linguaggio: dal corpo alla parola, trad. it., Roma, Borla, 1980.

ANZIEU D. (1985), Le Moi-peau, Paris, Dunod (L’io-pelle, trad. it., Roma, Borla, 1987).

ANZIEU D. (1990), L'épiderme nomade et la peau psychique, Paris, Apsygée (L’epidermide nomade e la pelle psichica, trad.it., Milano, Raffaello Cortina, 1992).

ARBIB M. A., OZTOP E., ZUKOW-GOLDRING E. (2005), «Language and the mirror system: a perception/action based approach to communicative development», in Cognitie, Craie, Comportement/Cognition, Brain Behavior IX (3), pp. 239-272.

ARGYLE M. (1975), Bodily communication, Matheu, London (Il corpo e il suo linguaggio, trad. it., Bologna, Zanichelli, 1978).

AUCOUTOURIER, B. (2005), La méthode Aucouturier. Fantasmes d'action & pratique psychomotrice, De Boeck Supérieur (Il metodo Aucouturier. Fantasmi d’azione e pratica psicomotoria, trad. it., Milano, Franco Angeli, 2005).

AUSTIN, J. L. (1962), How to do things with words, Cambridge, Harvard University Press (Come fare cose con le parole, trad. it, Genova, Marietti, 1987).

BARSALOU L.W. (1999), «Perceptual symbol system», in Behavioural and Brain Science, 22, pp. 577-660.

BARSALOU L.W. (2008), «Grounded cognition», in Annual Review of Psychology 59 (1) pp. 617-645.

BATES E., CAMAIONI L., VOLTERRA V. (1975), «The acquisition of performatives prior to speech», in Merrill Palmer Quarterly, n. 21, pp. 205-226.

BATES, E., BENIGNI L., BRETHERTON I., CAMAIONI, L. & VOLTERRA V. (1979), The emergence of symbols: Cognition and communication in infancy, New York, Academic Press.

BATES E., FREDERIC D. (2002), «Language, gesture and the developing brain», in CASEY B.J., MUNATAKA Y. (a cura di), Special Issue: Converging method approach to the study of developmental science, Developmental Psychobiology, n. 40, pp. 293-310.

BATES, E. (1979), «Intentions, conventions, and symbols», in BATES, E., BENIGNI L., BRETHERTON I., CAMAIONI, L., & VOLTERRA V. (1979), The emergence of symbols: Cognition and communication in infancy, New York Academic Press, pp. 33-68.

BERGEN B., MARGHETIS T. (2014), «Embodied meaning inside and out: the coupling of gesture and mental simulation», in MUELLER C., CIENKI, FRICKE, LADEWIG, MCNEILL, TEBENDORF, (a cura di), Body, Language, Communication, De Gruyter, pp.2000-2007.

BICK E. (1968), «L’esperienza della pelle nelle prime relazioni oggettuali», in BONAMINO V., IACCARINO B., (a cura di), 1984, L’osservazione diretta del bambino, Torino, Bollati Boringhieri, pp. 90-96.

BION W. R. (1962), Learning from Experience, London, William Heinemann Medical Books (Apprendere dall’esperienza, trad. it., Roma, Armando, 1972).

BLOOM K. (2006), The embodied self, London, Karnac.

BROWN, R. (1977), «Introduction», in SNOW C. E., FERGUSON C.

A. (a cura di), Talking to children. Language input and acquisition, Cambridge University Press, pp. 1-27.

BRUNER J. S., (1978), «Dalla comunicazione al linguaggio: una prospettiva psicologica», in CAMAIONI L., Sviluppo del Linguaggio ed Interazione Sociale, Bologna, Il Mulino, pp.75-112.

BRUNER J. (1995), «Costruzione di sé e costruzione del mondo», trad. it. in SEMPIO LIVERTA O., MARCHETTI A. (a cura di), Il pensiero dell’altro, Milano, Cortina, pp. 125-162.

BRUNER J. S., SHERWOOD V. (1988), «Pensiero, Linguaggio ed Interazione Sociale nell’infanzia», in UGAZIO V., La costruzione della conoscenza: l’approccio europeo alla cognizione del sociale, Milano, Franco Angeli, pp. 72-100.

CAPIRCI, O., CONTALDO, A., CASELLI, M. C., VOLTERRA, V. (2005), «From Action to language through gesture: a longitudinal perspective», in Gesture, n. 5, pp. 155-177.

CAPIRCI, O., VOLTERRA V. (2008), «Gesture and speech: the emergence and development of a strong changing partnership», in KENDON A., RUSSO T., Dimensions of Gestures, Special Issue 8:1, pp. 22-44.

CASELLI, C., MARAGNA S., VOLTERRA V. (2006), Linguaggio e sordità, Bologna, Il Mulino.

COSTE J. C. (1977), La psychomotricité, Presses Universitaires de France (La psicomotricità, trad. it., La Nuova Italia, Scandicci, 1981).

CUCCIO V. (in stampa), «Embodied simulation as a bodily attitude. For a direct role of the body in language and Cognition», in International Journal of Philosophical Studies.

DAMASIO A. (1994), Descartes’ Error: Emotion, Reason, and the Human Brain, New York, Putnam’s Sons (L’errore di Cartesio, trad. it., Milano, Adelphi, 1995).

DAMASIO A.(1999), The Feeling of What Happens. Body and Emotion in the Making of Consciousness, Orlando, Harcourt (Emozione e coscienza, trad.it., Adelphi, Milano, 2000).

DAMASIO A. (2010), Self Comes to Mind: Constructing the Conscious Brain, New York, Knopf Doubleday (Il Sé viene alla mente , trad. it., Milano, Adelphi, 2012).

DOLTO F. (1984), L’Image Inconsciente du Corps, Paris, Le Seuil (L’immagine inconscia del corpo, trad. it., Como, RED, 1996).

EKMAN P., FRIESEN W. V. (1969), «The repertoire of nonverbal behaviour: categories, origins, usage and coding», in Semiotica, n. 1, pp. 49-98.

FALK D. (2009), Finding Our Tongues. Mothers, Infants & the Origins of Language, Basic Books, New York (Lingua madre. Cure materne e origini del linguaggio, trad. it., Torino, Bollati Boringhieri, 2011).

FONTANA S., VOLTERRA V., (2012), «Gesti e Segni: Un’origine comune?», in FONTANA S., MIGNOSI E. (a cura di), Segnare, Parlare e Intendersi: Modalità e Forme, Milano, Mimesis, pp 109-133.

GALLESE V., LAKOFF G. (2005), «The brain’s concepts: the role of the sensory motor system in conceptual knowledge», in Cognitive Neuropsychology, 21, pp. 455–479.

GALLESE V., SINIGAGLIA C. (2011), «What is so special about embodied simulation», in Trends in Cognitive Science, n. 15(11), pp. 512-519.

GOODWYN S. W., ACREDOLO L.P. (1998), «Encouraging symbolic gestures: A new perspective on the relationship between gesture and speech», in IVERSON J., GOLDINMEADOW S. (a cura di), The Nature and Function of Gesture in Children’s Communications. New for Child Development, San Francisco, Jossey Bass,. 79, pp. 61-73.

IVERSON J., GOLDIN-MEADOW S. (1998), «Why People gesture when they speak», in Nature, n. 396, p. 228.

IVERSON J.M., THELEN E. (1999), «Hand, Mouth and Brain: the dynamic emergence of speech and gesture», in Journal of Conscoiusness Studies, 6, No 11-12, pp.19-40

IVERSON J., FAGAN (2004), «Infant Vocal-Motor Coordination: Precursor to the GestureSpeech System?», in Child Development, 75, n. 4, pp 1053-1066.

IVERSON J., GOLDIN-MEADOW S. (2005), «Gesture Paves the Way for Language Development», in American Psychological Society, vol. 16, n. 5, pp. 367-371.

KESTENBERG J.S (1975), Children and parents: psychoanalitic studies in development, New York, Jason Aronson.

KESTENBERG J.S., BUELTE A. (1977), «Prevention, infant therapy and treatment of adults: toward understanding mutuality», in International Journal of Psychoanalytic Psychotherapy, n. 6, pp. 338-367.

KIMURA D. (1981), «Neural Mechanism in Manual Signing», in Sign Language Studies, n. 33, pp. 291-311.

MASATAKA N. (2000), «The Role of Modality and Input in the Earliest Stage of LanguageAcquistion: Studies of Japanese Sign Language», in CHAMBERLAIN C., MORFORD J.P., MAYBERRY R., Language Acquisition by Eye, Mahwah, NJ, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

MCNEILL D. (1992), Hand and Mind: What gestures Reveal about Thought, Chicago, The University of Chicago Press.

MCNEILL D. (2005), Gesture and Thought, Chicago, The University of Chicago Press.

METZINGER T., GALLESE V. (2003), «The emergence of a shared action ontology: Building Blocks for a theory», in Consciousness and Cognition, n. 12, pp. 549-571.

MIGNOSI E. (2007), «Corpo movimento e stati psichici: l’osservazione del non verbale dei bambini», in BONDIOLI A. (a cura di), L’osservazione in campo educativo, Bergamo, Junior, 2007, pp. 97-124

OJEMANN, G.A. (1984), «Common Cortical and thalamic mechanisms for language and motor functions», American Journal of Physiology, n. 246 (Regulatory Integrative and Comparative Physiology 15, R901- R903).

PETTENATI, P., STEFANINI, S., &VOLTERRA, V. (2010), «Motoric characteristics of representational gestures produced by young children in a naming task», in Journal of Child Language, n. 37 (4), pp. 887-911.

PIAGET J. (1946), La formation du symbole chez l’enfant, Delachaux et Niestlé, Neuchâtel (La formazione del simbolo nel bambino, trad. it., Firenze, La Nuova Italia, 1991).

PIAGET J., INHELDER B. (1966), La psychologie de l’enfant, Paris, Presse Universitaire de France (La psicologia del bambino, trad.it., Torino, Einaudi, 1970).

PÜLVERMUELLER F., PREISSL H., LUTZENBERGER W., BIRBAUMER N. (1996), «Brain rythms of language: Nouns versus verbs», in European Journal of Neuroscience, n. 8, pp. 937-941.

PÜLVERMUELLER F., FADIGA L., «Active perception: sensorimotor circuits as a cortical basis for language», in Nature Reviews Neuroscience, n. 11 (5), pp. 351-360.

REDDY V. (2008), How Infants Know Minds, Harvard, Harvard University Press (Cosa passa per la testa di un bambino. Emozioni e scoperta della mente, trad. it. Milano, Raffaello Cortina, 2010).

RIZZOLATTI G., SINIGAGLIA C. (2006), So quel che fai. Il cervello che agisce e i neuroni specchio, Milano, Raffaello Cortina.

SCHORE A.S. (1994), Affect regulation and the origin of the Self: the neurobiology of emotional development, Hillsdale, NJ, Lawrence Erlbaum.

SNOW C. E. (1982), «Conversation with children», in FLETCHER P. & GARMAN M. (a cura di), Language acquisition. Studies in first language development, Cambridge, Cambridge University Press, pp. 69-89.

STERN D. N (1981), Le interazioni madre-bambino nello sviluppo e nella clinica, trad. it. Torino, Bollati Boringhieri, 1989.

STERN D. N. (1985), The interpersonal world of the infant, New York, Basic Books (Il mondo interpersonale del bambino, trad. it., Torino, Bollati Boringhieri, 1987).

STERN D.N. (2010), Forms of Vitality, Oxford, Oxford University Press (Le forme vitali, trad. it., Milano, Raffaello Cortina, 2011).

THELEN E. (1981), «Kicking, rocking and waving: Contextual analyses of rhythmical stereotypies in normal human infants», in Animal Behaviour, n. 29, pp. 3-11.

VOLTERRA V. & ERTING C. (1990), From Gesture to Language in Hearing and Deaf Children, Berlin, Springer Verlag.

WALLON H. (1972), The Origins of Character among Children (L’origine del carattere nel bambino, trad. it., Roma, Editori Riuniti, 1974).

WATZLAWICK P, BEAVIN J, JACKSON D. (1967), Pragmatica della comunicazione umana, ,trad. it., Roma, Astrolabio, 1971.

WINNICOTT D.W. (1958), Through Pediatrics to Psychoanalysis, London, Tavistock, (Dalla Pediatria alla Psicoanalisi, trad. it., Firenze, Martinelli, 1975).

WINNICOTT D.W. (1965), The maturational process and the facilitating environment, New York, International UP, Sviluppo affettivo e ambiente, trad. it., Roma, Armando, 1974.

WINNICOTT D.W. (1971), Playing and Reality, London, Tavistok Publications (Gioco e realtà, trad. it., Roma, Armando, 1974).

ZANI B., SELLERI P., DAVID D. (1996), La comunicazione, Roma, NIS

Published
2014-09-30
How to Cite
Fontana, S. and Mignosi, E. (2014) “Il gesto come risorsa bio-linguistica”, Rivista Italiana di Filosofia del Linguaggio, 00. Available at: http://rifl.unical.it/index.php/rifl/article/view/217 (Accessed: 21November2024).
Section
Sezione 3. Linguaggio e rito